What do students really understand from representations?

One thing that’s always bothered me about chemistry is the amount of information that’s hidden away in seemingly innocuous words or diagrams. For example, if I use the word ‘hydrogen’, do I mean a hydrogen atom or a molecule? Is it just one molecule or am I talking about a cylinder-full?

Mechanisms are even more complicated. There are lines for representing a pair of electrons and there are other lines for showing movement of a pair of electrons. Then sometimes we don’t even bother showing the electrons.

Chemistry is difficult because it is full of representations.

How students reason

Imagine a student presented with a reaction scheme, something we use to represent a chemical reaction. If the student considers only the functional groups or reagents to understand how the reactants turn into the products, then they would be doing so using perceptual attributes only – a surface level interpretation of the information. But if the student considers the mechanism or formation of intermediates, they would be using relational attributes – a much deeper level of interpretation showing they can relate the representation to its meaning.

shutterstock 58579726

Lots of chemical information can be hidden in representations

A recent study probed the perceptual and relational attributes that introductory chemistry undergraduate students use to categorise chemical reactions.

A recent study1 probed the perceptual and relational attributes that introductory chemistry undergraduate students use to categorise chemical reactions.

The researchers focused on a small subset of all possible chemical reactions: electrophilic addition to alkenes. These included reactions with dipolar, radical, cycloaddition and heterogeneous catalysis mechanisms. This restriction avoided cognitive overload for students while allowing enough area of study for meaningful observations.

They interviewed students to capture detailed information about their thoughts when presented with four different sets of reaction schemes.

Firstly, they were asked to classify a set of reactions with a common propene starting material by whatever attributes they thought appropriate and into as many categories as they wanted. Then they were asked to do the same with a set containing more varied alkene starting materials. They were questioned again on the first set, but now specifically asked to categorise by reaction mechanism. And finally, they were asked to rationalise their categorisation by mechanism.

The students’ interview responses were analysed based on their use of perceptual or relational attributes to explain their categorisation of each set of reaction schemes.

Blinded by perception

Given the complexities of a topic laden with hidden meaning, it’s not surprising that around three quarters of students’ categorisations were based solely on perceptual attributes. This suggests little chemical understanding was employed as they thought about the reactions presented to them.

The most notable perceptual attribute that was used was the similarity of the products based on their functional groups. The regiochemical similarity of products was also heavily used in the categorisation process. Although not as frequently mentioned, similarity between reagents used, such as acids or halogens, was also employed as a perceptual attribute for categorisation.

Students only formed just over 15% of their categories using relational attributes, ie mechanistic reasoning. Reasoning included the involvement of a carbocation intermediate, whether or not a reaction proceeds in a concerted fashion, or a molecule being involved in syn-addition.

The tendency to consider perceptual over relational attributes persists even when students were asked to use the mechanism as the basis for forming categories for the reactions.

It is not surprising that students choose to focus on the atomic level changes rather than the underlying mechanisms – it significantly reduces their cognitive load. However, this focus on perceptual attributes is probably our fault – textbooks and organic chemistry curricula are often organised by the chemistry of functional groups, elevating the importance of perceptual attributes at the outset.

Teaching tips

While it might not be possible to change the overall structure of a curriculum, you can promote an understanding of the meaning behind reaction schemes by altering how content is presented. Organising the content around mechanistic processes and reaction intermediates may help anchor cause and effect relationships and allow students to recall them in novel situations.

Students in this study used the Markovnikov (or anti-Markovnikov) rule as a purely perceptual attribute. This was often cited without any understanding of how the relative stability of intermediates directs the regiochemical outcome. If you were to teach this without initial reference to a named rule, and only through an exploration of the mechanism, perhaps students would understand the process more deeply.

Finally, as with most programmes of learning, carefully designed tasks can ensure the desired learning. In the context of this study, that would be promoting students’ understanding of relational attributes rather than perceptual ones. For this, the authors’ research instruments, found in the paper, could be directly employed or adapted for use in a classroom.

Reference

  • 1. N Graulich and G Bhattacharyya, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., DOI: 10.1039/c7rp00055c